[1] Csibra G. Recognizing communicative intentions in infancy[J].Mind Lang,2010,25(2):141-168.
[2] Lloyd-Fox S,Széplaki-Köllöd B,Yin J,et al.Are you talking to me? Neural activations in 6-month-old infants in response to being addressed during natural interactions[J].Cortex,2015,70(7):35-48.
[3] Hernik M,Broesch T. Infant gaze following depends on communicative signals: an eye-tracking study of 5- to 7-month-olds in Vanuatu[J].Dev Sci,2019,22(4):1-8.
[4] Silvia R,Menon E,Johnson MH,et al.Direct gaze may modulate face recognition in newborns[J].Infant Child Dev,2011,20(1):20-34.
[5] Ma W,Fiveash A,Margulis EH,et al.Song and infant-directed speech facilitate word learning[J].Q J Eep Psychol,2020,73(7):1036-1054.
[6] Zhang D,Roche L,Bartl-Pokorny KD,et al.Response to name and its value for the early detection of developmental disorders: Insights from autism spectrum disorder,Rett syndrome,and fragile X syndrome. A perspectives paper[J].Res Dev Disabil,2018,82: 95-108.
[7] Arslan M,Warreyn P,Dewaele N,et al.Development of neural responses to hearing their own name in infants at low and high risk for autism spectrum disorder[J].Dev Cogn Neuros-Neth,2020,41:100739.
[8] Nyström P,Bölte S,Falck-Ytter T. Responding to other people's direct gaze: alterations in gaze behavior in infants at risk for autism occur on very short timescales[J].J Autism Dev Disord,2017,47(11):3498-3509.
[9] Filipe MG,Watson L,Vicente SG,et al.Atypical preference for infant-directed speech as an early marker of autism spectrum disorders? A literature review and directions for further research[J].Clin Linguist Phonet,2018,32(3):213-231.
[10] Grossmann T. The development of social brain functions in infancy[J].Psychol Bull,2015,141(6):1266-1287.
[11] Cooper RP,Aslin RN. Preference for infant-directed speech in the first month after birth[J].Child Dev,1990,61(5):1584-1595.
[12] Minagawa Y,Xu M,Morimoto S. Toward interactive social neuroscience:neuroimaging real-world interactions in various populations[J].Jpn Psychol Res,2018,60(4):196-224.
[13] Farroni T,Csibra G,Simion F,et al.Eye contact detection in humans from birth[J].PNAS,2002,99(14):9602-9605.
[14] Guellaï B,Hausberger M,Chopin A,et al.Premises of social cognition: Newborns are sensitive to a direct versus a faraway gaze[J].Sci Rep,2020,10(1):1-8.
[15] Imafuku M,Hakuno Y,Uchida-Ota M,et al."Mom called me!" Behavioral and prefrontal responses of infants to self-names spoken by their mothers[J].Neuro Image,2014,103:476-484.
[16] Outters V,Schreiner MS,Behne T,et al.Maternal input and infants' response to infant-directed speech[J].Infancy,2020,25(4):478-499.
[17] Bigelow AE,Rochat P. Two-month-old infants' sensitivity to social contingency in mother-infant and stranger-infant interaction[J].Infancy,2006,9(3):313-325.
[18]Floccia C,Christophe A,Bertoncini J. High-amplitude sucking and newborns: the quest for underlying mechanisms[J].J Exp Child Psychol,1997,64(2):175-198.
[19] Mandel DR,Jusczyk PW,Pisoni DB. Infants' recognition of the sound patterns of their own names[J].Psychol Sci,1995,6(5):314-317.
[20] Newman RS. The cocktail party effect in infants revisited: listening to one's name in noise[J].Dev Psychol,2005,41(2):352-362.
[21] Okumura Y,Kanakogi Y,Kobayashi T,et al.Ostension affects infant learning more than attention[J].Cognition,2020,195:104082.
[22] Saito Y,Aoyama S,Kondo T,et al.Frontal cerebral blood flow change associated with infant-directed speech[J].Arch Dis Child-Fetal,2007,92(2):F113-F116.
[23] Saito Y,Kondo T,Aoyama S,et al.The function of the frontal lobe in neonates for response to a prosodic voice[J].Early Hum Dev,2007,83(4):225-230.
[24] Naoi N,Minagawa-Kawai Y,Kobayashi A,et al.Cerebral responses to infant-directed speech and the effect of talker familiarity[J].Neuro Image,2012,59(2):1735-1744.
[25] Grossmann T,Johnson MH,Lloyd-Fox S,et al.Early cortical specialization for face-to-face communication in human infants[J].Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci,2008,275(1653):2803-2811.
[26] Urakawa S,Takamoto K,Ishikawa A,et al.Selective medial prefrontal cortex responses during live mutual gaze interactions in human infants: an fNIRS study[J].Brain Topogr,2015,28(5):691-701.
[27] Grossmann T,Parise E,Friederici AD. The detection of communicative signals directed at the self in infant prefrontal cortex[J].Front Hum Neurosci,2010,4:201.
[28] Parise E,Csibra G. Neural responses to multimodal ostensive signals in 5-month-old infants[J].PLoS One,2013,8(8):e72360.
[29] Piazza EA,Hasenfratz L,Hasson U,et al.Infant and adult brains are coupled to the dynamics of natural communication[J].Psychol Sci,2020,31(1):6-17.
[30] Kampe KKW,Frith CD,Frith U. "Hey John": Signals conveying communicative intention toward the self activate brain regions associated with "mentalizing," regardless of modality[J].J Neurosci,2003,23(12):5258-5263.
[31] Háden GP,Mády K,Török M,et al.Newborn infants differently process adult directed and infant directed speech[J].Int J Psychophysiol,2020,147:107-112.
[32] Pelphrey KA,Mccarthy VG. When strangers pass: processing of mutual and averted social gaze in the superior temporal sulcus[J].Psychol Sci,2004,15(9):598-603.
[33] Pelphrey KA,Singerman JD,Allison T,et al.Brain activation evoked by perception of gaze shifts: the influence of context[J].Neuropsychologia,2003,41(2):156-170.
[34] Hakuno Y,Hata M,Naoi N,et al.Interactive live fNIRS reveals engagement of the temporoparietal junction in response to social contingency in infants[J].Neuro Image,2020,218:116901.
[35] 杨帅,黄希庭,傅于玲. 内侧前额叶皮质——“自我”的神经基础[J].心理科学进展,2012,20(6):853-862.
[36] Rochat P. Five levels of self-awareness as they unfold early in life[J].Conscious Cogn,2003,12(4):717-731.
[37] Rochat P. The self as phenotype[J].Conscious Cogn,2011,20(1):109-119. |